The Psychology of Selfishness: Evolutionary, Personality and Sociocultural Factors Driving Selfish Tendencies

Dr. Donna L. Roberts

“I say let the world go to hell, but I should always have my tea.” 
― Fyodor Dostoevsky, Notes from Underground

Selfishness, commonly defined as the act of prioritizing one's own needs and desires above those of others, has long been a topic of interest within psychology. While selfish behavior is often portrayed negatively, it is important to understand the various factors that contribute to its development and manifestation.

Evolutionary Roots of Selfishness

The foundation of selfishness can be traced back to the basic principles of evolution. From an evolutionary standpoint, the primary objective of any organism is to ensure the survival and reproduction of its genes (Dawkins, 1976). This inherently self-serving process promotes the development of traits and behaviors that enhance an individual's chances of success, often at the expense of others (Trivers, 1971). While cooperation and altruism have also evolved as adaptive strategies (Axelrod & Hamilton, 1981), selfishness remains a significant aspect of human behavior, as it can provide short-term benefits and promote self-preservation (de Waal, 2008).

Cognitive and Emotional Aspects

At the cognitive level, selfishness is closely related to the concept of egocentrism, which refers to the tendency to view the world from one's own perspective and prioritize one's own needs and desires (Piaget, 1954). Egocentrism is a natural part of human development, with children often exhibiting selfish behaviors until they develop the ability to understand other people's perspectives (Flavell, 1999).

Emotionally, selfishness has been linked to the experience of entitlement and narcissism. Entitlement involves the belief that one deserves special treatment or rewards, often resulting in the prioritization of one's own needs above those of others (Campbell et al., 2004). Narcissism, on the other hand, is characterized by an inflated sense of self-importance and a constant need for admiration, which can lead to selfish behaviors (Raskin & Terry, 1988).

Personality Traits and Selfishness

Research has identified various personality traits that are associated with increased levels of selfishness. The Dark Triad of personality, which includes Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy, has been found to be positively correlated with selfish behavior (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). Individuals high in these traits tend to manipulate others for personal gain, exhibit a lack of empathy, and prioritize their own interests above others (Jonason et al., 2011). Additionally, low levels of agreeableness, one of the Big Five personality traits, have been linked to selfish behaviors (Graziano et al., 1996). People who score low on agreeableness are typically less compassionate, cooperative, and trustworthy, which can result in a greater propensity for selfishness (Costa & McCrae, 1992).

Social Factors and Selfishness

The manifestation of selfishness is not solely determined by individual factors; social and environmental factors also play a significant role. For example, the presence of competition and scarce resources can elicit selfish behavior as individuals strive to secure their own well-being (Insko et al., 1993). Furthermore, cultural factors and social norms can also influence the expression of selfishness. Cultures that prioritize individualism and personal success may foster an environment in which selfish behaviors are more accepted and prevalent (Hofstede, 1980).

Interventions and Strategies to Mitigate Selfishness

One potential approach to mitigating selfishness is through promoting empathy and perspective-taking. Interventions that encourage individuals to consider the feelings and needs of others have been shown to reduce selfish behaviors and increase prosocial tendencies (Batson et al., 2002). Similarly, cultivating gratitude and promoting a focus on shared goals and values can also contribute to a reduction in selfish tendencies (Emmons & McCullough, 2003).

Furthermore, creating environments that foster cooperation and trust can help to counteract the effects of competition and scarcity that often promote selfishness (Ostrom, 1990). By establishing social norms that emphasize community and interdependence, it may be possible to encourage individuals to prioritize the collective good over personal gain (Fehr & Fischbacher, 2003).

Education and public awareness campaigns can be employed to challenge cultural factors that contribute to selfishness. By promoting cultural values that prioritize empathy, cooperation, and compassion, we can work towards creating a society in which selfish behavior is less prevalent and socially acceptable (Haidt, 2001).

The psychology of selfishness is a complex and multifaceted phenomenon, with roots in evolutionary processes, cognitive and emotional development, personality traits, and social factors. Understanding the various aspects of selfishness can provide valuable insights into human behavior and inform strategies for promoting prosocial behaviors and reducing negative outcomes associated with excessive selfishness.

References

Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211(4489), 1390-1396. DOI:10.1126/science.7466396

Batson, C. D., Ahmad, N., Lishner, D. A., & Tsang, J.-A. (2002). Empathy and altruism. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 485–498). Oxford University Press.

Campbell, W. K., Bonacci, A. M., Shelton, J., Exline, J. J., & Bushman, B. J. (2004). Psychological entitlement: interpersonal consequences and validation of a self-report measure. Journal of Personality Assessment83(1), 29–45. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa8301_04

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and Individual Differences, 13(6), 653–665. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(92)90236-I

Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford University Press.

de Waal F. B. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology59, 279–300. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625

Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 377–389. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.377

Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425(6960), 785-791. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02043

Flavell J. H. (1999). Cognitive development: children's knowledge about the mind. Annual Review of Psychology50, 21–45. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.21

Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Hair, E. C. (1996). Perceiving interpersonal conflict and reacting to it: The case for agreeableness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(4), 820–835. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.820

Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814

Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Sage Publications.

Insko, C. A., Schopler, J., Drigotas, S. M., Graetz, K. A., Kennedy, J., Cox, C., & Bornstein, G. (1993). The Role of Communication in Interindividual-Intergroup Discontinuity. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 37(1), 108–138. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022002793037001005

Jonason, P.K., Kavanagh, P.S., Webster, G.D., & Fitzgerald, D. (2011). Comparing the Measured and Latent Dark Triad: Are Three Measures Better than One? Journal of Methods and Measurement in the Social Sciences, 2, 28-44.

Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the commons: The evolution of institutions for collective action. Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511807763

Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(6), 556–563. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6

Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. Basic Books.

Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 890–902. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.890

Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1), 35-57.

This is original content from NewsBreak’s Creator Program. Join today to publish and share your own content.

Comments / 1

Published by

Writer and university professor researching media psych, generational studies, addiction psychology, human and animal rights, and the intersection of art and psychology.

Canandaigua, NY
4K followers

More from Dr. Donna L. Roberts

Comments / 0